They both belong to three-year-olds, so why is one so much bigger?
Because one was loved by its parents and the other neglected – a fact that has
dramatic implications.
Take a careful look at the image of two brains on this page. The picture
is of the brains of two three-year-old children. It’s obvious that the brain on
the left is much bigger than the one on the right. The image on the left also
has fewer spots, and far fewer dark “fuzzy” areas. To neurologists who study
the brain, and who have worked out how to interpret the images, the difference
between these two brains is both remarkable and shocking. The brain on the
right lacks some of the most fundamental areas present in the image on the
left.
Those deficits make it impossible for that child to develop capacities
that the child on the left will have: the child on the right will grow into an
adult who is less intelligent, less able to empathise with others, more likely
to become addicted to drugs and involved in violent crime than the child on the
left. The child on the right is much more likely to be unemployed and to be
dependent on welfare, and to develop mental and other serious health problems.
What could possibly cause so radical a divergence in brain development?
The obvious answer is that it must have been some illness or terrible accident.
The obvious answer is wrong. The primary cause of the extraordinary difference
between the brains of these two three-year-old children is the way they were
treated by their mothers. The child with the much more fully developed brain
was cherished by its mother, who was constantly and fully responsive to her
baby. The child with the shriveled brain was neglected and abused. That
difference in treatment explains why one child’s brain develops fully, and the
other’s does not.
Neurologists are beginning to understand exactly how a baby’s
interaction with their mother determines how, and indeed whether, the brain
grows in the way that it should. Professor Allan Schore, of UCLA, who has
surveyed the scientific literature and has made significant contributions to
it, stresses that the growth of brain cells is a “consequence of an infant’s
interaction with the main caregiver [usually the mother]”. The growth of the
baby’s brain “literally requires positive interaction between mother and
infant. The development of cerebral circuits depends on it.”
Prof Schore points out that if a baby is not treated properly in the
first two years of life, the genes for various aspects of brain function,
including intelligence, cannot operate, and may not even come into existence.
Nature and nurture cannot be disentangled: the genes a baby has will be
profoundly affected by the way it is treated. The details of how the chemical
reactions that are essential to the formation of new brain cells and the
connections between them are affected by the way a mother interacts with her
baby are extremely technical. Suffice it to say that there is now a very substantial
body of evidence that shows that the way a baby is treated in the first two
years determines whether or not the resulting adult has a fully functioning
brain.
The damage caused by neglect and other forms of abuse comes by degrees:
the more severe the neglect, the greater the damage. Eighty per cent of brain
cells that a person will ever have are manufactured during the first two years
after birth. If the process of building brain cells and connections between
them goes wrong, the deficits are permanent.
This discovery has enormous implications for social policy. It explains
two very persistent features of our society. One is the way that chronic
disadvantage reproduces itself across generations of the same families. There
is a cycle of deprivation – lack of educational attainment, persistent
unemployment, poverty, addiction, crime – which, once a family is in it, has
proved almost impossible to break.
The way that the development of a child’s brain is dependent on the way
that the child is treated by its mother explains why this depressing cycle
happens. Parents who, because their parents neglected them, do not have fully
developed brains, neglect their own children in a similar way: their own
children’s brains suffer from the same lack of development that blighted their
own lives. They, too, are likely to fail at school, to be liable to get
addicted to drugs, to be unable to hold down a job, and to have a propensity to
violence.
The second persistent feature is the dismal failure of rehabilitation programmes
that aim to diminish the rate at which persistent young offenders commit
crimes. Many different approaches have been tried, from intensive supervision
to taking young offenders on safaris, but none has worked reliably or
effectively. Recent research indicates that a large majority – perhaps more
than three quarters – of persistent young offenders have brains that have not
developed properly. They have, that is, suffered from neglect in the first two
years of life, which prevented their brains from growing. As a consequence,
they may be incapable of responding to the same incentives and punishments that
will steer those with more fully developed brains away from crime.
That result may lead you to conclude that nothing can be done about the
social problems that result from childhood neglect. But that would be wrong.
There is a way to break the cycle, and it is not terribly difficult to achieve.
It consists in intervening early and showing mothers who neglect their children
how to treat them in a way, which will lead their babies’ brains to develop
fully. “Early intervention”, as the policy is called, has been tried in parts
of the US for more than 15 years. It consists in ensuring that mothers
identified as “at risk” of neglecting their babies are given regular visits (at
least once every week) by a nurse who instructs them on how to care for the
newborn child. Data from the city of Elmira in New York State, where such
programmes have been in place longest, show that children whose mothers had
received those visits did much better than children from a comparable
background whose mothers were not part of the programme: they had, for
instance, 50 per cent fewer arrests, 80 per cent fewer convictions, and a
significantly lower rate of drug abuse.
Graham Allen, the Labour MP for Nottingham North, has been a fervent
advocate of introducing early intervention programmes into the UK since at
least 2008. That year, he collaborated with Iain Duncan Smith, now Secretary of
State for Work and Pensions, on Early Intervention: Good Parents, Great Kids,
Better Citizens, a report for the Centre for Social Justice which set out
evidence that the neglect of children in the first two years of life damages
the development of their brains. The report also looked at the social problems
that resulted, and examined the effects that early intervention could have in
helping to solve those problems.
Mr Allen’s own constituency is one of the most deprived in England: it
has the highest rates of teenage pregnancy in Europe, and one of the lowest
rates of participation in higher education. “There is no doubt that early
intervention can make a tremendous contribution to improving our society,” Mr
Allen says. “Not the least benefit is the financial one. The amount it saves
taxpayers, by reducing benefits, by cutting care home places for kids who would
otherwise have to be taken from their parents, by reducing prison places, and
so on, is staggering.”
Andrea Leadsom, the Conservative MP for South Northamptonshire, agrees.
She is a passionate advocate of early intervention programmes. “I know they
work because I have seen them in operation”, she says. “I helped to run an
early intervention centre in Oxford, one of the first early intervention
programmes in England. I have helped to institute such programmes in
Northamptonshire. I can bear witness to the astonishing benefits. The biggest
problem at the moment is that the programmes are far too small. In Oxford, the
centre sees perhaps 300 babies a year. But there are 17,000 babies born in Oxford
every year, which means there are 34,000 babies in Oxford in the first two
years of life who might benefit from the programme. We need central Government
to get behind early intervention so that it happens on a big enough scale
everywhere.”
Frank Field, the Labour MP for Birkenhead, is another passionate
advocate of early intervention. He has also introduced small-scale schemes in
his own constituency, and is working hard to find ways to get such schemes
adopted more widely. There is a remarkable cross-party consensus that early
intervention is a vitally important policy which needs to be supported
nationally. Both David Cameron and Ed Miliband have endorsed early
intervention, and insisted that it should be implemented.
But nothing is happening to make sure that it is. “Quite the opposite,”
notes Mr Allen. “The funding I thought was earmarked for it is being taken
away. The plans that I have put forward are being hollowed out.” “It’s crazy,”
adds Mrs Leadsom. “This is a policy that has the potential to transform our
society, to mean that the next generation of babies will grow into more
responsible, less crime-prone, and better educated adults. We know what needs
to be done to get those results: we need to ensure that mothers who are at risk
of neglecting or abusing their babies in the first two years of life are
instructed how to care for them and interact with them properly. But no one in
central government is pushing it. In fact, they’re taking away the early
intervention grant in order to pay for the pupil premium for two-year-olds.”
Frank Field is just as depressed about the prospects of getting early
intervention adopted by the Government. “The Prime Minister asked me to write a
report on early intervention,” he says. “My hopes were up when I delivered it
several weeks ago. But as far as I can tell, he hasn’t even read it.”
What explains the failure to adopt early intervention programmes
nationally? The greatest obstacle may simply be that the biggest benefits will
not be obvious for 15 years. The babies who benefit from early intervention
today will take more than a decade to grow into teenagers who do not commit the
crimes they would have perpetrated had their mothers not been helped by an
early intervention programme. Elections, however, are every five years. That
means the benefits will not accrue to the politicians in power now, but to
their successors – which could be why those in power now are reluctant to
expend effort and money on early intervention programmes. “I hope that isn’t
true,” says Graham Allen. “Because if it is, it would mean we are politically
incapable of implementing the one policy that will certainly make our society
immeasurably better. And what more profound condemnation of our political
system could there be than that?”
Source: Telegraph UK
Please share
No comments:
Post a Comment